Study: New Caledonian Crows can create compound tools

New Caledonian Crow
A New Caledonian Crow holds a tool. Photo by Auguste von Bayern

Scientists have known for some time that the New Caledonian Crow, a species found only on two islands in the South Pacific, is able to use and make tools in the wild and in captivity. Now, researchers from the Max Planck Institute for Ornithology and the University of Oxford have revealed that the species can create tools by combining two or more otherwise non-functional elements, an ability so far observed only in humans and great apes.

In a new study, published today in Scientific Reports, the scientists show that the birds can create long-reaching tools out of short, combinable parts — an astonishing mental feat. Assemblage of different components into novel functional and maneuverable tools has, until now, only been observed in apes, and anthropologists regard early human compound tool manufacture as a significant step in brain evolution. Children take several years before creating novel tools, probably because it requires anticipating properties of yet unseen objects. Such anticipation, or planning, is usually interpreted as involving creative mental modelling and executive functions.

The study demonstrates that the crows possess highly flexible abilities that allow them to solve complex problems involving anticipation of the properties of objects they have never seen.

Advertisement

New Caledonian Crow is one of 50 species featured in our 2018 collector’s edition, A World of Birds

“The finding is remarkable because the crows received no assistance or training in making these combinations,” says Auguste von Bayern, from the Max Planck Institute for Ornithology and University of Oxford. “They figured it out by themselves.”

The most notable New Caledonian Crow, a bird known as Betty, became famous in 2002 as the first animal shown to be able to create a hooked tool by bending a pliable material.

The University of Oxford’s Alex Kacelnik says: “The results corroborate that these crows possess highly flexible abilities that allow them to solve novel problems rapidly, but do not show how they do it. It is possible that they use some form of virtual simulation of the problem, as if different potential actions were played in their brains until they figure out a viable solution, and then do it.”

The researchers presented eight crows with a puzzle box they had never encountered before, containing a small food container behind a door that left a narrow gap along the bottom. Initially, the scientists left some sufficiently long sticks scattered around, and all the birds rapidly picked one of them, inserted it through the front gap, and pushed the food to an opening on the side of the box. All eight birds did this without any difficulty. In the next steps, the scientists left the food deep inside the box but provided only short pieces, too short to reach the food. These short pieces could potentially be combined with each other, as some were hollow and others could fit inside them. In one example, they gave the birds barrels and plungers of disassembled hypodermic syringes. Without any help or demonstration, four of the crows partially inserted one piece into another and used the resulting longer compound pole to reach and extract the food. At the end of the five-step investigation, the scientists made the task more difficult by supplying even shorter combinable parts, and found that one bird particular bird, “Mango,” was able to make compound tools out of three and even four parts.

Although the authors explain that the mental processes by which the birds achieve their goals have not yet been fully established, the ability to invent a tool is interesting in itself. Few animals are capable of making and using tools, and also in human development the capacity only emerges late. While children start using tools reliably when they are about 18 months old, they only invent novel tools suited to solve a given problem reliably when they are at least five years old. Archaeological findings indicate that such compound tools arose only late in human cultural evolution (probably around 300,000 years ago in the Middle Paleolithic) and might have coevolved with planning abilities, complex cognition and language. The crows’ ability to construct novel compound tools does not imply that their cognitive mechanisms equal those of humans or apes, but it helps to understand the cognitive processes that are necessary for physical problem solving.

Watch a video of the crows creating compound tools

 

Read more

Researchers discover how crows manufacture hooked tools

Critically endangered crow shown to be a skilled tool use

 

Read our newsletter!

Sign up for our free e-newsletter to receive news, photos of birds, attracting and ID tips, and more delivered to your inbox.

Sign Up for Free